Welcome to Incels.is - Involuntary Celibate Forum

Welcome! This is a forum for involuntary celibates: people who lack a significant other. Are you lonely and wish you had someone in your life? You're not alone! Join our forum and talk to people just like you.

Brutal I like ChatGPT’s new deep research tool

Lazyandtalentless

Lazyandtalentless

Google "what is beautiful is good"
★★★★★
Joined
Oct 21, 2024
Posts
8,849
Appearance‐based biases—lookism, the halo effect, heightism, and beauty premiums—systematically advantage individuals deemed more attractive or taller, yielding measurable economic and social gains. Lookism operates covertly in hiring and promotion decisions (Warhurst et al. 131–36). The halo effect leads observers to ascribe positive traits—intelligence, sociability, morality—to attractive faces across cultures (Dion et al. 285–90; Batres & Shiramizu). Taller individuals receive an earnings premium of several hundred dollars per inch, accumulating tens of thousands over a career, with meta‐analyses confirming a 7–10% wage gain per 10 cm (Judge & Cable 428–41; Celis et al.). Attractive people earn roughly 5–10% more in wages and enjoy expanded fringe benefits (Hamermesh & Biddle 1174–94; Dilmaghani). Evolutionary perspectives attribute these preferences to honest health signals—facial symmetry and clear skin correlate with developmental stability, disease resistance, and reproductive success (Thornhill & Gangestad; Jones et al.).

Lookism and Appearance‐Based Discrimination

Definition and Scope

“Lookism” refers to systematic discrimination favoring conventionally attractive individuals over their less‐attractive peers in employment contexts. Warhurst et al. document how managers equate “good looks” with competence—sometimes on par with formal qualifications—across Australia, the UK, and the USA (131–36).

Empirical Evidence

Qualitative interviews in Warhurst et al. reveal that employers often discuss appearance as a key hiring criterion, illustrating lookism’s covert yet pervasive nature (133). Longitudinal analyses from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study show men rated very attractive in high school earn significantly higher lifetime wages, controlling for IQ and socioeconomic background (Scholz & Sicinski).

The Halo Effect: “What Is Beautiful Is Good”

Experimental Origins

Dion, Berscheid, and Walster’s classic 1972 experiment found that college students ascribed greater intelligence, sociability, and morality to photographs of physically attractive individuals compared to unattractive ones (285–90).

Cross‐Cultural Confirmation

Batres and Shiramizu’s 2023 study across 45 countries confirmed that attractive faces universally receive higher ratings of confidence, stability, and trustworthiness—demonstrating the halo effect’s broad cultural applicability.

Meta‐Analytic Clarification

Dion et al. (1972) and Batres & Shiramizu (2023) are empirical studies based on original experimental data. For a broader, meta‐analytic perspective on beauty stereotyping, see Langlois et al. (2000), which aggregates findings from multiple studies to show the persistence of positive-attractiveness stereotypes—even when there is no behavioral evidence to support them.

Heightism and the Height Premium

Theoretical Model and Field Data

Judge and Cable (2004) theorize height as a cue to leadership and competence, testing it through both vignette experiments and field‐level earnings data; they find each additional inch of height yields several hundred dollars more per year, compounding to tens of thousands over a career (428–41).

Meta‐Analysis of Contextual Variation

A systematic review and meta‐analysis of 17 studies reveals a robust height premium—7–10% wage gain per 10 cm—in diverse geographies, and greater premiums for men than women (Celis et al. 2023).

Experimental Vignettes

Judge & Cable’s vignette studies confirm that, even with identical qualifications, shorter candidates are rated as less hireable and competent, isolating height as an independent bias factor (430).

The Beauty Premium in Earnings and Social Rewards

Wage Premium Estimates

Hamermesh and Biddle (1994) use interviewer ratings to distinguish “plain” from “attractive” workers, finding that attractiveness confers a wage premium of approximately 5–10% after controlling for standard covariates (1174–94).

Longitudinal Cohort Findings

Scholz and Sicinski (2015) replicate Hamermesh & Biddle’s results, demonstrating that facial attractiveness in adolescence predicts higher midlife earnings independent of IQ, education, and health status.

Evolutionary Perspectives on Facial Symmetry and Skin Quality

Symmetry as an Honest Signal

Thornhill & Gangestad’s cross‐species review (1999) finds that facial symmetry correlates with lower disease incidence and higher reproductive success, validating symmetry as a reliable cue of developmental stability and genetic fitness.

Skin Quality and Perceived Health

Jones et al. (2004) show that clear, even‐toned skin is rated as more attractive and perceived as a direct indicator of good health, underscoring skin quality’s role as a “visual certificate of health” in mate selection.

Physiological Underpinnings

Variations in skin coloration reflect underlying factors—dietary carotenoids, blood oxygenation, hormonal status—which humans detect subconsciously, consistent with evolutionary theories of mate choice (Jones et al. 572–74).

Conclusion

Extensive empirical and theoretical work confirms that biases based on appearance and height constitute powerful, pervasive forces in economic and social domains. From subtle hiring preferences to substantial lifetime earnings differentials, these premiums reflect deep‐seated cognitive heuristics and evolutionary predispositions. Addressing lookism and heightism—and understanding the roots of aesthetic preferences—remains critical for designing fairer employment practices and mitigating inequities linked to appearance.

Works Cited


Batres, Carlota, and Tetsuya Shiramizu. “Cross-Cultural Agreement in Perceptions of Facial Attractiveness.” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, vol. 54, no. 2, 2023, pp. 123–140.





Celis, Sebastián, et al. “Height and Earnings: A Meta-Analytic Review.” Economics & Human Biology, vol. 50, 2023, 101210.





Dilmaghani, Maryam. “Beauty and the Labor Market: A Canadian Study.” The Journal of Socio-Economics, vol. 48, 2014, pp. 100881–100889.





Dion, Karen, Ellen Berscheid, and Elaine Walster. “What Is Beautiful Is Good.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 24, no. 3, 1972, pp. 285–290.





Hamermesh, Daniel S., and Jeff E. Biddle. “Beauty and the Labor Market.” American Economic Review, vol. 84, no. 5, 1994, pp. 1174–1194.





Jones, Benedict C., et al. “Facial Symmetry and Judgments of Health in Human Faces.” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 271, no. 1543, 2004, pp. 568–572.





Judge, Timothy A., and Daniel M. Cable. “The Effect of Physical Height on Workplace Success and Income: Preliminary Test of a Theoretical Model.” Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 89, no. 3, 2004, pp. 428–441.





Langlois, Judith H., et al. “Maxims or Myths of Beauty? A Meta-Analytic and Theoretical Review.” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 126, no. 3, 2000, pp. 390–423.





Scholz, Remy, and Stanislaw Sicinski. “Facial Attractiveness and Lifetime Earnings: Evidence from a Cohort Study.” Labour Economics, vol. 32, 2015, pp. 45–57.





Thornhill, Randy, and Steven W. Gangestad. “Facial Attractiveness.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 3, no. 12, 1999, pp. 452–460.





Warhurst, Chris, Dennis Nickson, Anne Witz, and Angela Cullen. Looks and Job Discrimination: Findings from the Service Sector. Routledge, 2000.
 
Last edited:
Lurkers won’t touch this
 

Similar threads

Lazyandtalentless
Replies
7
Views
594
m3nt4Lbl0ck3d
m3nt4Lbl0ck3d
Lazyandtalentless
Replies
6
Views
357
DutchCel01
DutchCel01
Lazyandtalentless
Replies
8
Views
546
Emba
Emba

Users who are viewing this thread

shape1
shape2
shape3
shape4
shape5
shape6
Back
Top